Web site logical path: [www.psy.gla.ac.uk] [~steve] [best] [this page]
Bloom's taxonomy originated in an attempt to make assessment more systematic, though it is expressed as being about different types of learning objectives. It is particularly useful, as intended, for help in designing tests e.g. MCQs (multiple choice questions), since we tend to assume that MCQs can only test rote learning, but with care you can test much "higher" kinds of learning. My own interest is in learning and teaching in HE (Higher Education). The commentaries at the end probably show that focus, and may not have the same force in relation to school (K12).
Bloom's taxonomies have since been revised, above all by Anderson, Krathwohl et al. (hereafter "A&K"), and this page's first aim is to summarise those revisions for reference. If you are going to use Bloom's taxonomy today, you need to be aware of, and probably to use, the revised ones. However there is naturally the question of how useful such an old idea (more than 50 years old) still is, so the second aim here is to sketch an evaluation of that.
N.B. The taxonomy is what Bloom is most often cited for; however more important for improving learning and teaching in HE is probably his Mastery Learning work.
1) Multi-dimensional Bloom: domains of knowledge: (Levels X domains) | ||||||||||
level | Old (Bloom) | Cognitive (new, A&K) | Affective | Affective2 (Norman Reid) | Psycho-motor | Psychomotor2 (e.g. dance) | Perceptual (me) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
6 | Evaluation | Creating | - | World view | - | Communicative movement | Create complex perceptions in others | |||
5 | Synthesis | Evaluating | Characterising by value or value concept | Values | Naturalisation | Skilled moves | Complex scene understanding | |||
4 | Analysis | Analysing | Organising and conceptualising | - | Articulation | Physical abilities | Connoisseurship, classify perceptions | |||
3 | Application | Applying | Valuing | Attitudes | Precision | Perceptual abilities | Active perceptual skills | |||
2 | Comprehension | Understanding | Responding | Beliefs | Manipulation | Fundamental movements | Recognise novel cases | |||
1 | Knowledge | Remembering | Receiving | Knowledge | Imitation | Reflexes | Recognise standard cases | |||
0 | Understanding language | Understanding language | Feeling | - | Moving | Moving | Sensing |
2) Multi-dimensional neo-Bloom: types of knowledge [Levels X types, for cognitive domain only] neoBloom scheme in old Bloom table format. | |||||
level | Processes (internal, mental) | Factual | Conceptual | Procedural (external behaviour) | Metacognitive |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
6 | Creating | Combine | Plan | Compose | Actualise |
5 | Evaluating | Rank | Assess | Conclude | Action |
4 | Analysing | Order | Explain | Differentiate | Achieve |
3 | Applying | Classify | Experiment | Calculate | Construct |
2 | Understanding | Summarise | Interpret | Predict | Execute |
1 | Remembering | List | Describe | Tabulate | Appropriate use |
3) A&K usual presentation of the 2 dimensions as a blank table to use in analysing a course | ||||||
Cognitive process dimension Increasing mental complexity of processing → | ||||||
Knowledge (type) dimension: ↓ increasing abstractness ↓ |
1 Remember |
2 Understand |
3 Apply |
4 Analyse |
5 Evaluate |
6 Create |
Factual | ||||||
Conceptual | ||||||
Procedural | ||||||
Metacognitive |
4) A&K example use The definitions of each objective etc. for the course being analysed are normally given in footnotes or a key to the table. | ||||||
Cognitive process dimension Increasing mental complexity of processing → | ||||||
Knowledge (type) dimension: ↓ increasing abstractness ↓ |
1 Remember |
2 Understand |
3 Apply |
4 Analyse |
5 Evaluate |
6 Create |
Factual | Objective1, Activity1, Assess3 |
|||||
Conceptual | Activity1 | |||||
Procedural | Activity3 | Objective3, Activity3, Assess1 |
||||
Metacognitive | Objective2, Activity3 |
5) A&K filled with verb-noun phrases | ||||||
Level: | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Processes (internal, mental): | Remembering | Understanding | Applying | Analysing | Evaluating | Creating |
Factual | Remember facts | Understand facts | Apply facts | Analyse facts | Evaluate facts | Create facts |
Conceptual | Remember concepts | Understand concepts | Apply concepts | Analyse concepts | Evaluate concepts | Create concepts |
Procedural (external behaviour) | Remember procedures | Understand procedures | Apply procedures | Analyse procedures | Evaluate procedures | Create procedures |
Metacognitive | Remember metacognitive items | Understand metacognitive items | Apply metacognitive items | Analyse metacognitive items | Evaluate metacognitive items | Create metacognitive items |
6) A&K filled with keywords invented by others This is a transposed version of table 2 | ||||||
Level: | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Processes (internal, mental): | Remembering | Understanding | Applying | Analysing | Evaluating | Creating |
Factual | List | Summarise | Classify | Order | Rank | Combine |
Conceptual | Describe | Interpret | Experiment | Explain | Assess | Plan |
Procedural (external behaviour) | Tabulate | Predict | Calculate | Differentiate | Conclude | Compose |
Metacognitive | Appropriate use | Execute | Construct | Achieve | Action | Actualise |
8) Definitions of processes or levels? -- my rationale / restatement of the levels i.e. of the first dimension N.B. A&K call the set of 6 levels the dimension of cognitive processes | |||
level | Processes, internal A&K redefinition of Bloom levels as internal mental processes |
Procedural, external Examples of A&K's Procedural knowledge type (external behaviour) |
New definition (me) |
7 | - | - | Create new categories, concepts, or rules |
6 | Creating | Compose | Generate a new complex case from old elements |
5 | Evaluating | Conclude | Weighted overall judgement of several given complex cases, relating elements |
4 | Analysing | Differentiate | Break down a given complex case into known elements |
3 | Applying | Calculate | Apply old knowledge to new cases (transfer across cases) |
2 | Understanding | Predict | Paraphrase (transfer across descriptions) |
1 | Remembering | Tabulate | Name, recall, and reproduce (elements, relationships) |
0 | - | - | Understanding language |
A&K however show the 6 levels horizontally, with the more complex to the right. Table 3 shows them this way.
Table 2 is intermediate: it shows the basic A&K 2-D scheme, but in the old orientation.
The A&K ordering of the levels is used in the various A&K tables above.
The last table (8) (which uses the A&K ordering) addresses a question of the processes. The A&K labels refer to internal, mental ("cognitive") processes. There are corresponding external behavioural tasks, which are examples of A&K's "procedural knowledge type", and which you might require learners to do for instance in an assessment: shown in col.3. A longer definition of what I imagine these intellectual operations really mean is offered in the last column.
Thus A&K are fully aware that language is at best a partial clue to classifying
objectives. In their main vignettes they often discuss this, and end by
re-classifying an ILO from where they had first assigned it. Against this is
a strong desire many of us feel to summarise every category by a single
mnemonic word label -- specially useful when you are scanning a whole table
trying to get an overview, to reflect on contrasts and coverage. This web
page is primarily motivated by a desire to find or create such a useful visual
overview, and it leads to a use of 1-word labels. This is however dangerous,
problematic, and leads to many errors. A&K did NOT do this.
Metacognition
For A&K, the knowledge type of metacognition is not the aspect of
metacognition about self-regulation, not about managing one's learning; but
is knowledge about the knowledge, about managing the application of each bit
of knowledge. E.g. for arithmetic division, you can divide any two numbers
except you mustn't divide by zero; the part-whole skill training issue that
there is extra knowledge in how to connect the parts; near and far transfer
issue is that there is extra knowledge in how to apply knowledge to each new
case/context.
They subdivide it into:
One model for reviewing use of techno in teaching is the SAMR model (Substitution; Augmentation; Modification; Redefinition).
Putting SAMR together with neo-Bloom you get Bloom's Digital Taxonomy:-
Web page; or a 5-min video ...
which first reviews the Bloom taxonomy; then the changes which the neo-Bloom revised version makes; and then how to link that to modern technology in education. |
Johnstone's categorisation of types of problem-solving subverts it in another way. Problem-solving is close to the heart of assessment tasks in science, but instead of classifying them as a developmental sequence with 6 levels, Johnstone suggested there were 8 types (only 2 of which were widely seen in teaching), made up of all combinations of 3 binary properties: whether the data were given or incomplete; whether the outcomes or goals of the task were given or "open" (decided by the learner before or during the task); and whether the methods were familiar or unfamiliar.
Types of problem for solving (Johnstone 1993) | ||||
Type | Data | Methods | Outcomes/goals | Skills bonus |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 | Given | Familiar | Given | Recall of algorithms. |
2 | Given | Unfamiliar | Given | Looking for parallels to known methods. |
3 | Incomplete | Familiar | Given | Analysis of problem to decide what further data are required. Data seeking. |
4 | Incomplete | Unfamiliar | Given | Weighing up possible methods and then deciding on data required. |
5 | Given | Familiar | Open | Decision making about appropriate goals. Exploration of knowledge networks. |
6 | Given | Unfamiliar | Open | Decisions about goals and choices of appropriate methods. Exploration of knowledge and technique networks. |
7 | Incomplete | Familiar | Open | Once goals have been specified by the student these data are seen to be incomplete. |
8 | Incomplete | Unfamiliar | Open | Suggestion of goals and methods to get there; consequent need for additional data. All of the above skills. |
Johnstone,Alex. (1993) "Introduction" in Creative problem solving in chemistry: Solving problems through effective groupwork (London: Royal Society of Chemistry)
It is particularly useful for help in designing MCQs (multiple choice questions), since we tend to assume that MCQs can only test rote learning, but with care, you can test much "higher" kinds of learning.
The helpfulness is generally by associating each Bloom category with keywords and using these to prompt a teacher's imagination. This operation works with teachers who apply masses of commonsense when using the prompts. However the inverse operation works very badly: scanning text for the keywords and automatically classifying with the Bloom level, because English isn't simplistic like that.
In fact the "level" of a question actually depends on the processing it triggers in the learner's mind, and not on the words. Brain teasers (e.g. Mazur's "ConcepTests") have the surface form of simple factual questions, but trigger deep thinking about reasons for and against.
Steve Brindley's check list for learning designs (brindley@physics.gla.ac.uk) | ||
Intellectual aims and goals | Practical aims and goals | Attitudes and interests |
---|---|---|
Recall previous work | Lab observations | Individual / group work |
Problem recognition & awareness | Record data | Safety consciousness |
Problem solving | Communication/ discussion | Personal context |
Hypotheses, Prediction, Test | Using powers of observation | Practical problem solving. |
Generate ideas | Using equipment | Experimental learning |
Following practical instructions | ||
Recording facts / ideas in words | ||
Manipulating equipment |
Bloom taxonomy levels don't really work; particularly not for classifying test questions.
In fact the "level" of a question depends on the processing it triggers in the learner's mind, and not on the meanings of the words, let alone their surface form.
In fact, especially in HE, each discipline typically has a core test activity type e.g. essay writing in History, problem solving using calculations in physics. These are usually a fairly good match to the Bloom level demanded, inherently requiring high level cognitive functioning; although the highest marks may require additional functioning that is not explicitly required by the question i.e. the student must know the implicit demands of the discipline (see below) e.g. whether to display originality or not, whether to redefine the exam question or whether this will be penalised.
Here's where good examples would fit, referring to the "level of a question".
For instance: In school biology (since Bloom's time) it has become compulsory for pupils to learn to distinguish in photomicrographs and diagrams eukaryote from prokaryote cells; animal from plant cells, etc. A physician must be able to recognise a disease in a patient, not just talk about it to other medics. A student of chemistry should know what gold looks like compared to copper. These things are taught and assessed. Bloom's taxonomy misses their important place, and A&K perpetuate this major omission.
In using a checklist to review a course's content and assessment, I would always look at what connection it makes to students' personal experience, and especially to perceptual experience and skills.
The above are educational arguments why splitting motor from perception is a blunder. It was an old-fashioned psychological tradition to split them; but one that is regarded as wrong by some important cognitive approaches since Bloom e.g. J.J.Gibson, and work on robotics. You can't build a practicable robot by splitting them: Rodney Brooks argued and demonstrated that you have to have layers each of which have both sensing and motor action, distinguished by how fast (but stupidly) they can respond.
My impression is that there is no good work on this domain; and that defining a domain as motor without perception is a serious error both educationally and psychologically.
For instance, Arts schools (which at least in the UK award HE degrees), require for admission, in year 1, and in all exercises, that students exhibit different compositions from each other: i.e. they demand "creativity", the top Bloom level, pervasively. In contrast in psychology (for instance) students are drilled in an impersonal and conformist voice, not to put their own experiences forward as evidence, to produce the right answer not a creative one. Creativity is explicitly penalised.
The old Bloom hierarchy put Evaluation at the top, above synthesis. That aligned with the view that the best paid jobs were executives, filtering what was offered by underlings to select the best things for the bosses' needs. As in the Renaissance, artists were relatively low paid employees. A more romantic view, expressed in the revised taxonomy, puts a higher value on creativity. Yet this fails to distinguish the synthesis a plumber shows (selecting a novel combination of off-the-shelf pipe and joint elements to suit a unique need) from the creativity described in product design texts, where design is not about assembling existing components, but finding a design that optimises multiple conflicting demands (that the product be cheap, be functional, be easily assembled, be easily cleaned, .....).
In chemistry you will need to reproduce the symbol "W" for Tungsten, and paraphrasing that as "Wolfram" (which would demonstrate historical understanding) will be penalised: rote reproduction of the symbol is required. In many other disciplines however paraphrase will be a required demonstration. Not only does this show a difference in values between disciplines, but it would not be stated in test questions for chemistry: students must know this value.
History as a discipline can usefully be considered as a training in writing "the History essay". Students will eventually be expected to take the question, redefine it (and state and justify their own redefinition), relate it to theories (probably to more than one) thus demonstrating "applying" and "analysing", and produce a weighted evalution. But they will get the highest marks by arguing a hypothesis of their own (demonstrating "creating") although it would never be called that. However in many other disciplines, this would be penalised as "not answering the question". This is not because Bloom is right and all academics across the world are wrong: it is because there are different values in different discipines, and Bloom's taxonomy does not and can not represent this.
According to A&K, Bloom was aware that discipline area matters and said as much in writing. In that case, this criticism is not that they missed noticing the issue, but that their continued failure to address it does indeed undermine the whole enterprise.
What is bad about it includes the points above, such as its neglect of perceptual learning, and its denial that different discipines require different kinds of thinking. And that even if the taxonomy is valid, it is a taxonomy of invisible aspects of learners' minds, and we cannot systematically and reliably translate this into question formats or other concrete teaching and learning activities.
Its connection to improving teaching really amounts to a corrective for the pervasive tendency to design courses around "covering content", and instead prompts teachers to devote course time and effort to the mental tasks that are central to a given discipline: these are not merely higher types of cognitive processing than rote learning of answers, but more generally are what feedback and tutoring need to be concentrated on. (Content largely takes care of itself.) These are different in different disciplines, but what is common is the need to focus learning objectives, tests, and staff time on something more than reproduction of content.
Web site logical path:
[www.psy.gla.ac.uk]
[~steve]
[best]
[this page]
[Top of this page]