Last changed
3 May 2024 ............... Length about 2,500 words (18,000 bytes).
(Document started on 22 July 2019.)
This is a WWW document maintained by
Steve Draper, installed at http://www.psy.gla.ac.uk/~steve/best/dispositions.html.
You may copy it.
How to refer to it.
Web site logical path:
[www.psy.gla.ac.uk]
[~steve]
[best]
[this page]
Sets of dispositions (to actions)
By
Steve Draper,
Department of Psychology,
University of Glasgow.
Dispositions ("DtoAs"): not feelings but actions are their root nature.
Think of actions as resources.
The usual "commonsense", but faulty, view of instincts is that they correspond to one big
function or purpose or goal e.g. to reproduce / eat / escape.
Analogue "planning": choosing and organising actions is not always and only discrete or digital.
A. Dispositions to action ("DtoAs")
When people today think about emotions, they tend to think of them as
alternative kinds of feelings. In fact, it is probably more insightful to
think of them as alternative dispositions to action e.g. fear IS the
disposition to flee and/or hide.
The general idea is that we have inside us some standard responses or actions,
and an instant reaction to some event is to select one from a small set of
types of possible action.
B. Think of actions as resources
Or rather, think of DtoAs as resources for deciding plans and action on the
fly; and not as just dormant plan/software libraries.
Not large libraries, but small sets of alternatives, which are not at all fully
fixed but are initial tendencies.
Because the "libraries" are primarily decision-making resources,
rather than action resources i.e. careful, complex, pre-planned actions.
C. Evolutionary functions vs. DtoAs
The usual "commonsense" view of instincts is that they correspond to one big
function or purpose e.g. eating / self-protection / reproduction.
But the DtoAs are smaller. We as theorists may "explain" them as contributing
to joined up actions that deliver the big functions, but observations support
DtoAs much better.
D. Analogue "planning" not digital.
Each disposition may have its own level of activation, and fight it out with the
alternatives.
How likely we are to enact such a DtoA in our current situation varies,
partly with whether other emotions are pushing in another direction (anger is
often visibly balanced by fear).
Furthermore, the detailed implementation (plan) varies
according to situation too (will I strike them, throw things at them, try to
recruit supporters for a joint attack, think of something really hurtful to
say, ....).
And after that, we may start to think about the cost of each action: but the
preliminary shape of our response was determined very quickly, but very
roughly, by these ready-made small sets of dispositions to action.
The idea of a set of DtoAs is similar to Konrad Lorenz's view of instincts
in ch.6 of On Aggression.
This chapter was about the ways an animal's set of instincts can interact
with each other. Furthermore that each such instinct has some degree of
autonomous force.
And they may counteract each other; or in displacement activities, the "force"
of one drive may get expressed in a dysfunctional way e.g. you have a bad day
at work and are angry with your boss, then take it out by snapping at your
family and friends.
This suggests that in animals, including humans, planning is analogue
not digital; and amounts to quantitative balance, not exact pre-planning of
action.
E. The real heart of this web page is a shift in perspective
- Emotions are not primarily feelings but generic action types.
- Think of actions as resources, not fully prepared action plans.
- They are not libraries of fixed action sequences like software
subroutines, but resources for planning.
- Consider the large variations in specific actions still to be decided by
the person and explained by theorist in much of human behaviour.
Once this perspective is adopted, then you may start to notice that a number of
apparently quite separate areas of psychology can also be seen as sets of
dispositions to action. This page explores this little thought.
There are several different sets, quite different in their origins, that have
in common that they are sets of dispositions to action.
- Konrad Lorenz's account of the five standard actions
young ravens apply.
- Emotions
- Stages of grieving
- The five personality dimensions
Ravens' sequence of actions
I cannot now find something which I read long ago. I think it was by Konrad
Lorenz, but I might be wrong. I think it was ravens but it might have been
another species of corvid.
The observation as I remember it was that young ravens generally follow a fixed
sequence of responses when a new, strange object appears or is found by them.
- First they hide from it.
- Then they attack it.
- Then they play with it
- Then they try to eat it.
When the affordance of an object is NOT clear, then discovering it amounts to
trying out all your set of actions on the object in turn to see if any "work"
usefully.
You can see human infants put almost anything into their mouths; throw anything
out of their cots; ....
The lesson that has remained with me is of creatures who learn about objects
through a fixed set of actions which yield information as well as,
occasionally, material rewards.
Affordances cannot always be perceived directly; sometimes they can only be
learned through interaction (play, exploration).
Emotions
Human emotions can be seen as infinitely variable; but also can by many be
seen as consisting of a small fixed number. Each of these is not only a
feeling, nor a facial expression, but is also associated with a particular
class or type of action. Here I have arbitrarily picked a set of seven.
- Disgust: disposed to physically reject by moving away, vomiting, shunning;
avoid all touching. (But not to move far; and to move the face away first,
the whole body less far.)
- Contempt: (the social version of disgust):
the disposition towards the person who is the object of your contempt,
that you despise, do not value them.
- Joy: disposed to be excited, full of energy, but without any particular
purposeful action.
- Sadness: disposed to be quiet, not to act, show little inclination to
action of any kind. Be inert.
- Surprise: disposed to startle, to be very alert and seek more information;
but not to move much. (The opposite of being bored, and disinclined to
pay attention to any one thing.)
- Anger: disposed to attack and hurt the person or thing.
Run towards or throw things at.
- Fear: disposed to flee, avoid, hide from, seek shelter from the person or
thing. Run away.
Grieving
Stages of grieving (and of other things)
- Denial
- Anger
- Bargaining
- Despair
- Acceptance
These are all attitudes that dictate types of action, or predispose toward one
type. The fact that typically a person does not move through these in a
strict and predictable sequence only reinforces the realisation that they are
possible types of action, not part of a fixed, rational plan.
Personality
Page on the golden mean
The most widely validated model of personality identifies five independent
dimensions. A given person has particular amounts of each of these five.
These dimensions are frequently labelled with a single term e.g. "openness";
but actually, it is more helpful to recognise that average people will have a
value mid-way between two opposite values; thus the "openness" dimension is
actually one that runs between openness and close-mindedness.
This bipolar view was the one Aristotle had in his model of virtues,
leading to the term
the golden mean.
His example was that the virtuous soldier exhibits the golden mean between
cowardice and foolhardiness.
- Openness ↔ traditional thinker.
Progressive ↔ traditional thinker
High value for imagination ↔ high value for experience.
Choose action on what can create a better future OR based on the
past only.
- Conscientiousness ↔ Irresponsible.
High impulse control ↔ low impulse control.
Attention to external measures of achievement ↔
attention to internal (personal) measures.
Doing what was asked of you ↔
doing what seems actually best to you.
- Agreeableness: Cares a lot about pleasing others ↔ indifferent
to displeasing others.
- Extraversion ↔ Intraversion.
Roughly: highly sociable ↔ small appetite for socialising.
Prefers interaction with people, even if only for its own sake.
(Perhaps) Keen to align / synchronise with others' actions
↔ prefers solo activity.
Or perhaps: Driven by external cues from others ↔
driven by internal needs and goals.
- "Neuroticism": Easily worried ↔ Calm, hard to upset.
Catastrophisation
"Catastrophisation" is a name for what some people do when, as soon as they
hear of some symptom or event, they instantly put the worst possible
interpretation on it. But if we think of this as a disposition to select fear
as the guiding principle for interpreting events and their impending
consequences, then there could be corresponding "mental reflexes" for each
emotion.
I.e. perhaps there are variants on catastrophisation, where the person
instantly interprets a new event as meriting that disposition.
- Fear: Traditional "catastrophisation": the person assumes the worst
possible interpretation, thus justifying being fearful of it.
- Sadness: interprets the event as bad; despair, hopelessness; "We're all
doomed! Doomed!".
- Contempt: cynicism. The person assumes the interpretation of an event is
evidence of despicable actions by others.
Contempt is the social version of disgust for objects.
- Disgust: similar. But turning away from things and people.
An interpretation of an event as justifying your abandoning something.
- Anger: interprets the event as something to be angry about; "blame culture".
- Joy: disposed to be excited, full of energy, but without any particular
purposeful action.
- Surprise: It's all so important! Amazing!
"Wow" as the default reaction to any event.
Not alternative dispositions, but stages
Related yet contrasting are things like
the 6 (or 12) steps
to recover from addiction.
While the sets of dispositions are each a set of alternative actions,
the 6 steps are steps all of which must be accomplished to achieve
recovery. You need all of them, not to pick one from the set depending on the
situation.
This is similar to CBT -- cognitive behavioural therapy. The therapy and the
client must tackle and change both the cognitions (thoughts) and
the behaviours; and contrary to what was commonly thought, these are not
alternative therapies.
Most human action is under-determined
Simple determination: one action, one reason for it
Ordinary things in life are usually done for one reason only e.g. I drop into
the local newsagent on, but only on, the days I want to buy the TV listings or
a bit of stationery.
Over-determination
Freud noted, however, that the really big things in life
such as choosing whom you marry, what house you will live in, what job/career
you will pursue are done for multiple reasons (are "over-determined"), so that
if one of those reasons later fails, it may very well not mean changing that
decision. Such actions are selected AND performed for multiple strong
reasons.
Under-determination
But a different observation we should note is that a lot of human
action is under-determined: that knowing why a person is doing an action,
often (usually) gets nowhere near letting you predict exactly what they do.
I need to get to work most days: that is why I do the journey. But
I might choose my method of travel to get some exercise, to pass through a
green space, to be able to chat to my friend regularly by sharing the mode of
transport (even if it isn't the very quickest way for me to travel).
And this is just one tiny example of the under-determination of human actions
by understanding only the main goal of the action.
To execute most actions entails taking many more decisions about methods which
are not determined by the main goal. This is both a fact about planning
in general, and a psychological fact about how human planning seems to
work, and a fact about how human behaviour has so much variability in it.
Just some of the main types of under-determination of human actions
- Speed vs. error rate
The great majority of tasks we set ourselves do not have a fixed timescale nor
error rate. Generally, speed and error rate are inversely proportional
for a given task, so a choice is somehow made.
See this section
for a dramatic example of a 4-fold increase in worker speed. But also consider
how differently you do a task when trying to go as fast as possible,
vs. to avoid errors as much as possible.
This is also strongly related to the effect of others' expectations as in the
Pygmalion effect of school teachers' expectations on
actual pupil performance.
Knowing what is to be done often has little to tell you about
how it will be done.
- Which disposition-to-action is selected
given the initially available information? (See "catastrophisation" above.)
Even what is to be done may vary depending not on objective facts about
the situation but on major differences in how the actor initially perceived
the situation.
- All actions have multiple effects, and you may (or may not) select
a particular action (from a set of all the actions that all achieve the
intended effect) on the basis of gaining or avoiding secondary (i.e. not
primarily intended) effects.
- All human actions have two main types of effect: those that change
the state of the material world; and those that change (increase) what the actor
knows. Normally we either do an action for the material effect and neglect
the fact that we will learn something (get better at the skill, and perhaps
also perceive something while doing it); or do an action to see what happens
(i.e. learn some new fact) and ignore the fact that we also wear out shoes
looking over the hill or round the corner; get tired, strain our eyes, and
spend a whole day when reading a book in order to learn something.
- Roles. What people do, and how they do it, are strongly affected
by the role they adopt and display, not only by their private goals. If you
don't know what role they are playing, you are unlikely to be able to predict
or understand their actions. This is true in the work place; in how parents
behave with their families; and just as much in how players behave in video
games, where their adopted character frequently changes their behaviour
greatly.
[Read Goffman on roles, and particularly on his concept of "role distance".]
Tony Clear's work
Tony.Clear@aut.ac.nz Auckland University of Technology
https://academics.aut.ac.nz/tony.clear
https://academics.aut.ac.nz/tony.clear/publications
Tony Clear has proposed that "collective empathy" is a skill or ability which
is highly desirable in a Software Engineer working in a team (as most
programmers do outside the amateur world).
He has worked out how to specify this in the language of current educational
curriculum specs. In that language:
Competencies =
{Knowledge + Skills + Dispositions} in the context of a Task.
Furthermore he suggested that
empathy might have three aspects: {cognitive, affective, behavioural}.
This work is partly expressed in a 2024 paper:
Computing Competencies, Dispositions and the Affective Taxonomy: More Work
Still to Do?
So this page is not only some notes on concepts, and how some concepts are
linked philosophically and conceptually to other concepts normally thought of
differently, but may well be a contribution to ideas and concepts of practical
technical importance in education.
Web site logical path:
[www.psy.gla.ac.uk]
[~steve]
[best]
[this page]
[Top of this page]